Ann Rehabil Med Search

CLOSE


Ann Rehabil Med > Volume 48(2); 2024 > Article
Lee, Kim, Lee, and Lee: Establishing Reference Values for a New Computerized Cognitive Function Test Program for Children

Abstract

Objective

To establish reference values for the computerized cognitive test and evaluate cognitive function improvements across different age groups, we introduce the computerized Cognitive Function Test program (eCFT), specifically designed for children. We aimed to establish eCFT reference values and assess cognitive function improvements across different age groups.

Methods

We included children aged 3–6 years with confirmed normal cognition based on the Korean Developmental Screening Test for Infants and Children and Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children-II. The eCFT consists of 8 subtests for visual perception, attention, memory, and executive function.

Results

A total of 66 participants (36 males and 30 females) with an average age of 4.4 years participated. The age 6 group consistently outperformed both age group 3 and 4 in terms of correct responses. With regard to the completed stage, the “selective auditory stimulus” test findings were 2.0 and 3.9 for the age 3 and age 6 groups, respectively (p<0.05). The “trail-making” test findings were 1.7, 2.1, 2.6, and 2.8, respectively (between ages 3 and 6, p<0.01; between ages 4 and 6, p<0.05); moreover, the age 5 group surpassed the age 3 group (2.6 and 1.7, respectively, p<0.05).

Conclusion

The eCFT is an easily accessible tool to evaluate cognitive function in young children. We introduce reference values with a cutoff range for preschool-aged children, enabling early intervention for those with cognitive impairment. Given its accessibility and relatively short evaluation time, the eCFT has potential for clinical use.

GRAPHICAL ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION

Cognitive function plays a crucial role in children’s independence in daily activities and social engagement [1]. Cognitive rehabilitation is necessary to treat children with cognitive impairment or developmental delays because it can prevent cognitive decline and reduce social costs [2,3]. The effectiveness of cognitive rehabilitation has been demonstrated in patients with cognitive impairments [4,5] and it has even been shown to improve cognitive function in children without overall learning difficulties [5]. Although cognitive rehabilitation can be achieved through pharmacological or non-pharmacological treatments, more careful consideration is needed when administering medications for children owing to the negative perception of caregivers and patients towards medications, and concerns about potential side effects. Additionally, there is more substantial evidence supporting the use of pharmacological intervention in adults, while the data regarding their efficacy and safety in children is insufficient [6-10]. However, traditional programs such as one-on-one or group therapy may be unavailable or impractical because of resource limitations and health conditions [11].
Computerized cognitive training (CCT) can compensate for these shortcomings, and several CCT programs, including RehaCom (HASOMED), ComCog (neofect), and Bettercog (M3 Solutions), have demonstrated effectiveness in enhancing cognitive function in individuals with cognitive impairment. RehaCom has shown its efficacy in improving cognitive function in children with ADHD and chronic stroke patients [12,13]. ComCog has demonstrated cognitive improvement in individuals with mild cognitive impairment (MCI), stroke, traumatic brain injury (TBI) patients [14,15]. In addition, ComCog has exhibited greater cognitive enhancement in a young TBI group than in an old TBI group [16].
The accurate evaluation of cognitive function is essential prior to rehabilitation. Computerized cognitive function tests serve as practical and accessible tools for this purpose, as they help reduce the examiner’s workload, minimize the variability introduced by human assessors, enable automated data storage, and capture reaction time data. Several Computerized Cognitive Test Batteries are available for children [17,18]. Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery is the most frequently used one and it is only applicable for children aged 4 years and above [18]. The National Institutes of Health Toolbox Cognition Battery and Amsterdam Neuropsychological Test are the only tests that can be applicable from the age of 3, while others can be employed for children aged 4–5 and above [18].
The computerized Cognitive Function Test program (eCFT) is a newly developed online cognitive function test that is part of the Bettercog (computerized cognitive rehabilitation program) developed in Korea. The eCFT is specifically designed to assess cognition in Korean children aged 3 and above, using words and images which are familiar to Korean children. It utilizes a touchscreen-based interface for user interactions. We, therefore, aimed to establish reference values for the eCFT and to investigate whether there is a trend of increasing cognitive function with age.

METHODS

Participants

We included children aged 3–6 years who were identified as having normal cognition through the Korean Developmental Screening Test for Infants and Children (K-DST) and Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children-II (K-ABC-II) test. Based on previous studies’ methodology, we aimed to recruit 15 subjects per age group [19, 20].
The participants were recruited via poster advertisements at the National Traffic Injury Rehabilitation Hospital and the study was conducted from November 2021 to September 2022. The exclusion criteria were hearing difficulties, visual impairments, hemi-neglect, and other musculoskeletal or neurological disorders that could impede following the instructions. The participants visited the research lab once to complete the K-DST, K-ABC-II, and eCFT. The assessments were conducted by a single occupational therapist, with a total duration of approximately 2 hours, including approximately 40 minutes for the eCFT.
The K-DST is a standardized questionnaire completed by caregivers to identify developmental delays in children. It evaluates various domains, such as motor skills, language, social and personal skills, and problem-solving ability [21,22]. It consists of six domains, each with a scoping range from 0 to 24 points. If a domain score is more than 1 standard deviation (SD) above the mean within the age group, it indicates a potentially higher-than-average developmental level. Scores within ±1 SD represent normal development. Scores below -1 SD and greater than -2 SD indicate a need for continuous observation. Scores below -2 SD suggest developmental delay, requiring further evaluation. The revised 2017 version was used in this study [23]. The K-ABC-II is a standardized test designed to measure cognitive ability and achievement in children and adolescents, to assess cognitive domains, including memory, planning, attention, and problem-solving abilities [24]. It comprises five domains and 18 subtests, with each subtest having a maximum scale score ranging from 1 to 19 points. Depending on the child’s age, subtests are selectively administered rather than conducting all 18 subtests on each child. Test results are interpreted using standard scores, with a mean of 100 and a SD of 15, and a maximum score of 160. Standard scores within ±1 SD are considered “average,” scores below -1 SD and greater than -2 SD are “low,” scores below -2 SD are “very low,” scores higher than +1 SD and below +2 SD are “high,” and scores higher than +2 SD are “very high.” The Korean version of the test was used to assess the cognitive function in this study [25].

Computerized program description

The eCFT consists of 8 subtests designed to evaluate various cognitive domains, including visual perception, attention, memory, and executive function. These subtests are: “finding half,” “visual and auditory stimulus,” “flip sequence,” “delayed recall,” and “trail-making.” Table 1 presents the instructions for each subtest. The “finding half” test was used to assess visual perception. The “visual and auditory stimulus” test evaluates attention and reaction time, while the “selective visual” and “selective auditory” stimulus tests measure complex attention and reaction times. The “flip sequence” test is designed to assess immediate memory and the “delayed recall” test focuses on delayed memory. The “trail-making” test is a tool to evaluate working memory.
Each subtest has unique characteristics and requirements. The “finding half” test requires participants to respond to each of the 20 questions within a 10-second timeframe. The “visual stimulus,” “selective visual stimulus,” “auditory stimulus,” and “selective auditory stimulus” tests are structured across 5 stages each. The “visual” and “auditory” stimulus tests present 5 stimuli at each stage, for a total of 25 questions, while the “selective visual” and “selective auditory stimulus” tests feature 3 true and false stimuli in each stage, resulting in a total of 30 questions. False stimuli were presented randomly, and the stimulation and interval times were modified to adjust the difficulty level of each stage. For the “visual” and “selective visual stimulus tests,” the stimulation times per stage were set to 4, 2.5, 2, 1.5, and 1 second. Unlike that in visual stimuli, it was challenging to maintain a constant stimulation time for auditory stimuli and it was adjusted only by the interval time. The interval time for all four tests was consistently set to 3, 2.5, 2, 1.5, and 1 second [26]. The “flip sequence” test comprises five stages, with a total of 20 questions. The interval time ranged from 3 to 5 seconds. Lower and higher stages were evaluated with a 3- and 5-second interval, respectively. This means that participants were required to remember the “flip sequence” for 3 to 5 seconds before recalling it, and they were required to answer each question within 10 seconds [27]. The “delayed recall” test consists of a single stage comprising 10 questions. Participants are shown two pictures for 5 seconds, followed by playing “Whack-A-Mole” for 10 seconds. Following this, they are required to recognize the previously displayed pictures from a set of five within the given 10 seconds. The trail-making test comprised 3 stages. In the first stage, the participants were instructed to click on the order of the rainbow colors. In the second stage, they were required to click from 1 to 7, and, in the final stage, they alternated between rainbow colors and numbers. The participants were required to respond to each stage within 2 minutes. The test time was shortened if the participants responded earlier than the given time or failed a stage.

Outcome measures

The data collected from the eCFT included the number of correct answers, incorrect answers, incomplete responses, completed stages, and reaction times.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using the IBM SPSS statistics 23 (IBM Corp.). Descriptive statistics, such as mean, SD, 15th percentile, and 85th percentile of the number of correct answers and reaction time, were presented. An analysis of variance was used to compare mean differences across age groups, and a post hoc Games–Howell test was conducted to determine specific mean differences. Statistical significance was set at p<0.05.

Ethics approval

This study adhered to the Declaration of Helsinki (2013 revision) and ICH-GCP guidelines. The study received approval from the Institutional Review Board of Seoul National University Hospital (IRB No. 2107-171-1236) and from the Institutional Review Board of National Traffic Injury Rehabilitation Hospital (IRB No. 2021-10-022). Informed consent was obtained from legally authorized representatives.

RESULTS

We included 66 participants (36 males) aged 3–6 years old (mean, 4.4 years old). None of the participants had a significant medical history and information regarding their socioeconomic status was not collected for this study.
The results of the K-DST and K-ABC-II are presented in Table 2. For the K-DST, the average score was 127.2±10.8. The total scores per age group were 128.9, 126.4, 124.9, and 137.5, respectively. In the K-ABC-II, the average score was 158.4±50.2. The total scores for each age group were 104.3, 143.9, 188.0, and 204.4. All participants scored within the normal range on the K-DST and K-ABC II tests.
Table 3 presents the results of the 8 eCFT subtests, including the number of participants, SD, 15th percentile, and 85th percentile for both the number of correct answers and the reaction time. A cutoff value at the 15th percentile, equivalent to 1 SD, was employed to identify abnormal performance.
The results demonstrate a progressive improvement in the number of correct answers with increasing age (Fig. 1). Improvements in the “selective visual stimulus,” “auditory stimulus,” “selective auditory stimulus,” “flip sequence,” and “trail-making” tests were observed in children aged 3–6 years. The age 6 group exhibited significantly superior performance compared to the age 3 and 4 groups in the “visual stimulus” test (age 3 group: 17.9±4.6, age 4 group: 21.4±4.0, and age 6 group: 24.6±1.1; between ages 3 and 6, p=0.001 and between ages 4 and 6, p=0.011). The age 6 group outperformed the age 3 group and the age 5 group achieved a higher score than the age 4 group in the “auditory stimulus” test (age 3 group: 14.5±5.8, age 4 group: 16.9±6.4, age 5 group: 19.9±5.2, and age 6 group: 23.8±1.9; between ages 3 and 6, p=0.001 and between ages 4 and 6, p=0.001). No disparities in performance across age groups were observed in the “selective visual” test, while the age 6 group outperformed the age 3 group in the “selective auditory” test (age 3 group: 4.4±2.3 and age 6 group: 13.3±8.6, p=0.010). In the “flip sequence” test, the age 6 group performed significantly better than the age 3 and 4 groups (age 3 group: 9.5±7.8, age 4 group: 11.5±8.0 and age 6 group: 22.8±7.6; between ages 3 and 6, p=0.001 and between ages 4 and 6, p=0.001), and in the “delayed recall” test, the age 6 group surpassed the performance of the age 4 group (age 4 group: 6.0±3.2 and age 6 group: 9.3±1.0. p=0.001). The age 6 group achieved higher scores than the age 3 and 4 groups in the “trail-making” test (age 3 group: 6.0±6.6, age 4 group: 10.2±8.7, age 5 group: 15.1±8.2, and age 6 group: 22.5±8.0; between ages 3 and 6, p<0.001 and between ages 4 and 6, p=0.002), while the age 5 group outperformed the age 3 group (p=0.017).
Although the reaction time tended to decrease as the children’s age increased, the difference was not statistically significant. Regarding the completed stages, the age 6 group completed more stages than the age 3 group in the selective auditory stimulus test. The average completed task stage was 2.0 for the age 3 group and 3.9 for the age 6 group (p<0.05; Fig. 2). The task completion rates for passing up to stage 5 were as follows: 10% for the age 3 group, 11% for the age 4 group, 14% for the age 5 group, and 57% for the age 6 group. In the “trail-making” test, the age 6 group completed more stages than the age 3 and 4 groups did, while the age 5 group completed more stages than the age 3 group did. The average completed task stage was 1.7 for the age 3 group, 2.1 for the age 4 group, 2.6 for age 5 group, and 2.8 for the age 6 group (between ages 3 and 6, p<0.01; between ages 4 and 6, p<0.05; and between ages 3 and 5, p<0.05). The task completion rates for passing up to stage 3 were as follow: 29% for the age 3 group, 47% for the age 4 group, 79% for the age 5 group, and 92% for the age 6 group.

DISCUSSION

We evaluated age-specific differences in eCFT results among children without cognitive impairment and proposed cutoff values based on the 15th percentile of the number of correct answers and reaction time for each subtest. The eCFT results indicated a pattern of increasing correct responses and decreasing reaction times with age. Notably, age 6 group consistently outperformed both age groups 3 and 4 in terms of correct responses. In the “selective auditory stimulus” test, the age 6 group demonstrated higher task completion rates compared to the age 3 group. In the “trail-making” test, the age 6 group completed more task stages than both the age 3 and age 4 groups. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to establish reference values for computerized cognitive assessments tailored specifically to preschool-aged children with typical cognitive development in Korea.
Our findings revealed that the mean performance scores in each cognitive domain showed slight variations depending on the child’s age, with older children tending to achieve higher scores. Notably, the age 6 group consistently outperformed the age 3 and 4 groups across various subtests. Furthermore, older groups tended to exhibit shorter reaction times in the attention domain, although this trend was not statistically significant. However, this pattern of shorter reaction times with increasing age was not observed in the findings of the “flip sequence” test. In other words, in challenging tasks such as memory function tests, younger children can exhibit shorter reaction times with lower accuracy than those exhibited by older children.
Furthermore, reaction time is directly related to attention. In a previous study, the average reaction time in the visual Continuous Performance Test (CPT) for typical Korean adults was reported as 0.42 seconds, while in the auditory CPT, it was 0.59 seconds [26]. In our study, the 6-year-old participants exhibited mean reaction times of 0.58 seconds in the “visual stimulus” test and 0.91 seconds in the “auditory stimulus” test, which differs from adults. The lower reaction time in the age 6 group compared to that of adults could be derived from either motor control or attention-related issues. Examining the results of the auditory stimulus test can further elaborate this. The average reaction time for visual and auditory controlled CPT (CCPT) in adults was reported as 0.41 and 0.57 seconds, respectively. Additionally, the average reaction times for the “selective visual stimulus” and “selective auditory stimulus” tests were 0.44 and 1.27 seconds, respectively, in the age 6 group. This suggests that inhibitory responses to auditory stimuli are somewhat more challenging than those to visual stimuli, and it implies that the difference observed in visual stimuli may be attention-related and attributed to processing speed rather than motor control [26]. In another study [19], a computerized attention assessment for children aged 5 to 15 years was developed, which was used to evaluate auditory CCPT and visual CCPT, dividing the total assessment time into three phases (early, middle, and late). The test provided target ratios of 22% in the early phase, 50% in the middle phase, and 78% in the late phase, and it maintained a fixed stimulus interval of 2 seconds and a stimulus presentation time of 0.1 seconds. The results revealed that the average reaction time for visual CCPT in 5- and 6-year-olds was 0.67 and 0.61 seconds, respectively, whereas our eCFT “selective visual stimulus” reported 0.61 and 0.44 seconds, respectively. For auditory CCPT, the average reaction time in 5- and 6-year-olds was 1.07 seconds, and our eCFT’s “selective auditory stimulus” test reported 1.40 and 1.27 seconds, respectively [19].
Regarding the completed stages, statistically significant differences were observed only in the “selective auditory stimulus” test and “trail-making” test. There were no differences in other attention-related subtests or the “flip sequence” test. This could be interpreted as indicating that executive function in 6-year-olds significantly outpaces that in other age groups, and that they exhibit superior sustained attention and better impulsivity control.
In addition, accurate responses to auditory stimuli are often lower than those to visual stimuli. In particular, the “selective auditory stimulus” test is challenging because of inhibition [19]. Our findings have corroborated this trend, revealing significant differences in the “selective auditory stimulus” among the completed stages.
The “trail-making” test assess motor speed, speed of mental processing and mental flexibility. It was used for children with learning disabilities and attention problems as well as those affected by brain injury [28]. In a study involving children aged 9 to 14, the alternative (number-letter) “trail-making” test was conducted, dividing participants into three groups: a group with brain injuries, a group with academic difficulties, and a control group. The test involved connecting 15 numbers and 15 letters alternatively. The completion times differed significantly, with the brain-damaged, academic difficulties, and control groups requiring 111.44, 68.38, and 33.56 seconds, respectively, to finish the test. [29]. As our study targeted children aged 3 to 6 years, the “trail-making” test used in our study utilized numbers 1–7 and colors (red, orange, yellow, green, blue, indigo, and violet). The average completion times of step 3 for the age 3, age 4, age 5, and age 6 groups were 68.54, 53.71, 44.73, and 36.17 seconds, respectively (not presented in Table). However, the number of individuals who completed the step 3 varied. In the age 3, age 4, age 5, and age 6 groups; 4/14, 9/19, 11/14, and 12/13 participants, respectively, completed the test. Even when examining normally developing children, there were instances where individuals did not successfully complete the alternative “trail-making” test. Therefore, considering these challenges, the use of the “alternating selection (step 3)” test might be more suitable for older age groups than that of the other test.
Computerized tests offer several advantages, including reducing the burden of examiners, minimizing variation by examiners, automatically storing data, obtaining information about reaction time, and allowing testing to be done at home [19], and have, therefore, gained popularity. The BMT-i, computerized Adaptable Test Battery to assess children’s academic skills and cognitive function has been validated [17].
Measuring reaction time is a good screening tool for normal aging and MCI, and it is more accurate than measuring correct answers in adults aged >60 years of age [30]. Previous studies established a correlation between processing speed and overall cognitive performance [31]. Notably, individuals diagnosed with ADHD exhibit challenges in sustaining attention and show significant impairments in selective attention as they have higher rates of omission and commission errors. In the case of unpredictable stimulus, children with ADHD tend to make a substantial number of those errors [32,33]
This study has certain limitations. First, it was conducted within a specific region of Korea, which potentially limits the generalizability of our results to other populations. Second, the small sample size may have contributed to the not significant differences between the groups. For instance, in the “selective visual” test, the age 6 group obtained considerably higher scores than the other groups (scores: 22.9, 24.6, 26.3, and 32.1 for ages 3, 4, 5, and 6, respectively), although this difference did not reach statistical significance. It is plausible that a larger sample size would reveal significant differences. Third, we did not collect detailed information about the patients’ characteristics, such as their socioeconomic status, number of siblings, and parental education levels. These factors may have influenced our results. Forth, we used colors (red, orange, yellow, green, blue, indigo, and violet) instead of letters in the “trail-making” test. This was done to avoid the possible unfamiliarity of very young children with letters. However, the test may not have been accurately conducted for children with color blindness, considering that we did not conduct a screening test for color blindness.
Using the eCFT, we established reference values for each subtest within the age range of 3–6 years, providing insights into the cognitive development of preschool children without cognitive impairment. Our findings indicated a progressive improvement in cognitive performance with age, which was particularly evident in specific subtests. The eCFT, known for its accessibility and cost-effectiveness, holds great promise for clinical application in identifying cognitive impairment and facilitating early intervention in preschool-aged individuals.
Future research should explore the potential of the eCFT as a tool to distinguish between children with and without cognitive impairments, such as those with low academic skills or attention deficits. Additionally, further investigations may seek to identify the age at which children’s scores on the eCFT begin to plateau in a specific cognition domain.

CONFLICTS OF INTEREST

The funding for this research was provided by M3 SOLUTIONS, CO., LTD, with Seoul National University Hospital serving as the lead research institution. This work falls under the category of commissioned research and the M3 Company played no role in the research design, data collection, analysis, or interpretation of results. The funding covered research expenses, equipment acquisition, and publication costs, with no influence on the research findings. The research was conducted independently, and the researchers maintained academic independence regarding the interpretation of the research results.

FUNDING INFORMATION

The funding for this research was provided by M3 SOLUTIONS, CO., LTD, with Seoul National University Hospital serving as the lead research institution (grant number: 0620214740). This work falls under the category of commissioned research.

AUTHOR CONTRIBUTION

Conceptualization: Lee GJ. Data curation: Kim H, Lee S. Methodology: Lee GJ. Formal analysis: Lee H. Funding acquisition: Lee GJ. Investigation: Lee H, Kim H. Project administration: Lee GJ. Resources: Lee GJ. Supervision: Lee GJ. Validation: Lee H. Visualization: Lee H. Writing – original draft: Lee H. Writing – review and editing: Lee H.

Fig. 1.
Comparison of cognitive function scores among age groups per subset. (A) Attention. (B) Memory. (C) Working memory. *p<0.05, **p<0.01, and ***p<0.001.
arm-230014f1.jpg
Fig. 2.
Comparison of completed stages among age groups by subsets. (A) Attention. (B) Memory. (C) Working memory. *p<0.05 and **p<0.01.
arm-230014f2.jpg
arm-230014f3.jpg
Table 1.
Computerized Cognitive Function Test program (eCFT) task description: 8 subtests
eCFT task Display Cognitive domain Test description
Finding half arm-230014i1.jpg Visual perception Half of a picture is presented. Find the corresponding half
Visual stimulus arm-230014i2.jpg Attention, reaction time Touch the screen when a picture is presented
arm-230014i3.jpg
Selective visual stimulus arm-230014i4.jpg Complex attention, reaction time Touch the screen when a specific picture appears and refrain from touching the screen when other pictures appear
arm-230014i5.jpg
Auditory stimulus arm-230014i6.jpg Attention, reaction time Touch the screen when a sound is heard
arm-230014i7.jpg
Selective auditory stimulus arm-230014i8.jpg Complex attention, reaction time Touch the screen when certain sounds are heard, and refrain from touching the screen when other sounds are heard
arm-230014i9.jpg
Flip sequence arm-230014i10.jpg Immediate memory Coins are flipped in a specific order. After a few seconds, participants are required to repeat the order of the flips
arm-230014i11.jpg
Delayed recall arm-230014i12.jpg Delayed memory Two pictures are presented. Play Whack-A-Mole for 10 seconds. Of the 5 pictures, select the 2 previously presented
arm-230014i13.jpg
arm-230014i14.jpg
Trail-making arm-230014i15.jpg Working memory Step 1. Click in order of rainbow colors
arm-230014i16.jpg Step 2. Click from 1 to 7
arm-230014i17.jpg Step 3. Click the rainbow color and number alternately
Table 2.
Demographic and clinical characteristics
Age (yr) N (female, %) K-DST K-ABC-II
3 15 (8, 53.3) 128.9±7.8 (117, 142) 104.3±16.9 (62, 127)
4 21 (7, 33.3) 126.4±12.1 (103, 140) 143.9±38.8 (104, 298)
5 16 (8, 50.0) 124.9±11.5 (100, 137) 188.0±34.6 (147, 269)
6 14 (7, 50.0) 137.5±9.2 (131, 144) 204.4±39.0 (143, 275)
Total 66 (30, 45.5) 127.2±10.8 158.4±50.2

Values are presented as mean±standard deviation (minimum, maximum).

K-DST, Korean Developmental Screening Test for Infants and Children; K-ABC-II, Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children-II.

Table 3.
Test results according to age group
Test Age (yr) N Number of correct answers Mean±SD Reaction time 
Mean±SD 15th % 85th % 15th % 85th %
Finding half 3 12 13.5±4.6 8.8 19.0 4.01±1.78 2.32 5.71
4 21 18.3±2.3 15.3 20.0 3.61±1.54 2.37 4.84
5 15 18.3±1.4 17.0 19.6 3.28±1.50 1.97 4.59
6 14 17.1±4.6 15.3 20.0 2.53±1.04 1.66 3.21
Visual stimulus 3 13 17.9±4.6a) 11.1 22.0 1.30±0.52 0.84 1.79
4 21 21.4±4.0b) 17.3 24.0 0.96±0.48 0.58 1.33
5 15 20.9±2.0 13.6 25.0 0.81±0.39 0.52 1.22
6 14 24.6±1.1a),b) 24.0 25.0 0.58±0.24 0.42 0.73
Selective visual stimulus 3 12 22.9±13.1 5.0 31.2 1.00±0.51 0.57 1.43
4 21 24.6±5.9 17.3 29.0 0.70±0.51 0.27 1.00
5 15 26.3±4.3 20.4 29.6 0.61±0.44 0.23 0.96
6 14 32.1±9.8 26.0 48.8 0.44±0.37 0.13 0.82
Auditory stimulus 3 11 14.5±5.8c) 6.6 22.0 1.14±0.65 0.40 1.73
4 21 16.9±6.4d) 7.6 23.0 1.14±0.53 0.64 1.61
5 15 19.9±5.2 12.6 24.2 1.08±0.52 0.63 1.53
6 14 23.8±1.9c),d) 20.8 25.0 0.91±0.38 0.61 1.28
Selective auditory stimulus 3 10 4.4±2.3e) 2.0 7.4 1.85±0.69 1.27 2.43
4 19 6.5±3.5 3.0 11.0 1.49±0.56 0.88 1.97
5 14 8.9±6.4 3.3 12.5 1.40±0.37 1.12 1.69
6 14 13.3±8.6e) 5.3 27.8 1.27±0.50 0.86 1.79
Flip sequence 3 14 9.5±7.8f) 1.0 20.5 4.48±2.41 1.79 7.25
4 20 11.5±8.0g) 1.2 21.9 3.34±2.10 1.38 5.32
5 14 15.0±10.9 1.3 28.8 3.49±2.06 1.51 5.53
6 14 22.8±7.6f),g) 12.8 28.8 3.71±2.18 1.63 5.74
Delayed recall 3 11 7.1±3.3 2.4 10.0 5.13±2.22 3.30 7.65
4 20 6.0±3.2h) 1.0 9.0 4.53±1.95 2.64 6.87
5 14 7.6±2.2 4.8 9.8 4.11±1.82 2.47 5.98
6 12 9.3±1.0h) 8.0 10.0 3.60±1.42 2.42 4.88
Trail-making 3 14 6.0±6.6i),k) 0 14.0 4.63±3.73 1.30 7.60
4 19 10.2±8.7j) 0 20.0 3.66±3.34 1.01 5.63
5 14 15.1±8.2k) 3.3 26.8 2.94±2.92 0.93 4.90
6 13 22.5±8.0i),j) 15.3 28.0 2.16±1.80 0.78 3.48

SD, standard deviation.

a)Visual stimulus test between age 3 and age 6, p=0.001.

b)Visual stimulus test between age 4 and age 6, p=0.011.

c)Auditory stimulus test between age 3 and age 6, p=0.001.

d)Auditory stimulus test between age 4 and age 6, p=0.001.

e)Selective auditory stimulus test between age 3 and age 6, p=0.010.

f)Flip sequence test between age 3 and age 6, p=0.001.

g)Flip sequence test between age 4 and age 6, p=0.001.

h)Delayed recall test between age 4 and age 6, p=0.001.

i)Trail making test between age 3 and age 6, p<0.001.

j)Trail making test between age 4 and age 6, p=0.002.

k)Trail making test between age 3 and age 5, p=0.017.

REFERENCES

1. Voorman JM, Dallmeijer AJ, Schuengel C, Knol DL, Lankhorst GJ, Becher JG. Activities and participation of 9- to 13-year-old children with cerebral palsy. Clin Rehabil 2006;20:937-48.
crossref pmid pdf
2. Laatsch L, Harrington D, Hotz G, Marcantuono J, Mozzoni MP, Walsh V, et al. An evidence-based review of cognitive and behavioral rehabilitation treatment studies in children with acquired brain injury. J Head Trauma Rehabil 2007;22:248-56.
crossref pmid
3. Chevignard M. Children with brain tumours need long-term multidisciplinary psychosocial, neurocognitive, academic and rehabilitation follow-up programmes. Acta Paediatr 2016;105:574-5.
crossref pmid
4. Bergquist T, Gehl C, Mandrekar J, Lepore S, Hanna S, Osten A, et al. The effect of internet-based cognitive rehabilitation in persons with memory impairments after severe traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj 2009;23:790-9.
crossref pmid
5. Klingberg T, Fernell E, Olesen PJ, Johnson M, Gustafsson P, Dahlström K, et al. Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD--a randomized, controlled trial. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2005;44:177-86.
crossref pmid
6. Takeda A, Loveman E, Clegg A, Kirby J, Picot J, Payne E, et al. A systematic review of the clinical effectiveness of donepezil, rivastigmine and galantamine on cognition, quality of life and adverse events in Alzheimer’s disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2006;21:17-28.
crossref pmid
7. Woods B, Aguirre E, Spector AE, Orrell M. Cognitive stimulation to improve cognitive functioning in people with dementia. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2012;(2): CD005562.
crossref pmid
8. Qiu H, Liang X, Wang P, Zhang H, Shum DHK. Efficacy of non-pharmacological interventions on executive functions in children and adolescents with ADHD: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Asian J Psychiatr 2023;87:103692.
crossref pmid
9. Sibley MH, Bruton AM, Zhao X, Johnstone JM, Mitchell J, Hatsu I, et al. Non-pharmacological interventions for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. Lancet Child Adolesc Health 2023;7:415-28.
crossref pmid
10. Pangilinan PH, Giacoletti-Argento A, Shellhaas R, Hurvitz EA, Hornyak JE. Neuropharmacology in pediatric brain injury: a review. PM R 2010;2:1127-40.
crossref pmid pdf
11. Kesler SR, Lacayo NJ, Jo B. A pilot study of an online cognitive rehabilitation program for executive function skills in children with cancer-related brain injury. Brain Inj 2011;25:101-12.
crossref pmid pmc
12. Mozaffari M, Hassani-Abharian P, Kholghi G, Vaseghi S, Zarrindast MR, Nasehi M. Treatment with RehaCom computerized rehabilitation program improves response control, but not attention in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). J Clin Neurosci 2022;98:149-53.
crossref pmid
13. Amiri S, Hassani-Abharian P, Vaseghi S, Kazemi R, Nasehi M. Effect of RehaCom cognitive rehabilitation software on working memory and processing speed in chronic ischemic stroke patients. Assist Technol 2023;35:41-7.
crossref pmid
14. Jung H, Jeong JG, Cheong YS, Nam TW, Kim JH, Park CH, et al. The effectiveness of computer-assisted cognitive rehabilitation and the degree of recovery in patients with traumatic brain injury and stroke. J Clin Med 2021;10:5728.
crossref pmid pmc
15. Kim S, Park E, Cha H, Jung JC, Jung TD, Chang Y. Effects of cognitive training in mild cognitive impairmentmeasured by resting state functional imaging. Behav Sci (Basel) 2020;10:175.
crossref pmid pmc
16. Kim SH, Gwak DW, Jeong JG, Jung H, Min YS, Kim AR, et al. Effect of computerized cognitive rehabilitation in comparison between young and old age after traumatic brain injury. Medicine (Baltimore) 2022;101:e29874.
crossref pmid pmc
17. Billard C, Thiébaut E, Gassama S, Touzin M, Thalabard JC, Mirassou A, et al. The Computerized Adaptable Test Battery (BMT-i) for rapid assessment of children’s academic skills and cognitive functions: a validation study. Front Pediatr 2021;9:656180.
crossref pmid pmc
18. Tuerk C, Saha T, Bouchard MF, Booij L. Computerized cognitive test batteries for children and adolescents-a scoping review of tools for lab- and web-based settings from 2000 to 2021. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2023;38:1683-710.
crossref pmid pdf
19. Shin MS, Cho S, Chun SY, Hong KM. A study of the development and standardization of ADHD diagnostic system. J Korean Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2000;11:91-9.

20. Ha KS, Kwon JS, Lyoo IK, Kong SW, Lee DW, Youn T. Development and standardization process, and factor analysis of the computerized cognitive function test system for Korea adults. J Korean Neuropsychiatr Assoc 2002;41:551-62.

21. Chung HJ, Yang D, Kim GH, Kim SK, Kim SW, Kim YK, et al. Development of the Korean Developmental Screening Test for Infants and Children (K-DST). Clin Exp Pediatr 2020;63:438-46.
crossref pmid pmc pdf
22. Kim D, Choe YJ, Durrani BAZ, Kim E, Byeon J, Eun BL. Korean Developmental Screening Test for Infants and Children (K-DST): development, applications, and implications for future early childhood development interventions. Clin Exp Pediatr 2023;66:288-93.
crossref pmid pmc pdf
23. Eun BL. Standardization and validity reevaluation of the Korean Developmental Screening Test for Infants & Children. Korea Centers for Disease Control and Prevention; 2017. p.196-225.

24. Kaufman AS, O’Neal MR, Avant AH, Long SW. Introduction to the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children (K-ABC) for pediatric neuroclinicians. J Child Neurol 1987;2:3-16.
crossref pmid pdf
25. Moon S. Korean Kaufman brief intelligence test. Inpsyt; 2020.p. 155.

26. Ha KS, Kwon JS, Lyoo IK. Development and standardization of the computerized attention assessment for Korean adults. J Korean Neuropsychiatr Assoc 2002;41:335-46.

27. Hilbert S, Nakagawa TT, Puci P, Zech A, Bühner M. The digit span backwards task: verbal and visual cognitive strategies in working memory assessment. Eur J Psychol Assess 2015;31:174-80.
crossref
28. Mittelmeier C, Rossi JS, Berman A. Discriminative ability of the Trail Making Test in young children. Int J Clin Neuropsychol 1989;11:163-6.

29. Reitan RM, Wolfson D. The Trail Making Test as an initial screening procedure for neuropsychological impairment in older children. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2004;19:281-8.
crossref pmid
30. Hartle L, Martorelli M, Balboni G, Souza R, Charchat-Fichman H. Diagnostic accuracy of CompCog: reaction time as a screening measure for mild cognitive impairment. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 2022;80:570-9.
crossref pmid pmc
31. Martorelli M, Hartle L, Coutinho G, Mograbi DC, Chaves D, Silberman C, et al. Diagnostic accuracy of early cognitive indicators in mild cognitive impairment. Dement Neuropsychol 2020;14:358-65.
crossref pmid pmc
32. Johnson KA, Robertson IH, Kelly SP, Silk TJ, Barry E, Dáibhis A, et al. Dissociation in performance of children with ADHD and high-functioning autism on a task of sustained attention. Neuropsychologia 2007;45:2234-45.
crossref pmid pmc
33. Fuermaier ABM, Tucha L, Guo N, Mette C, Müller BW, Scherbaum N, et al. It takes time: vigilance and sustained attention assessment in adults with ADHD. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2022;19:5216.
crossref pmid pmc


ABOUT
ARTICLE TYPES

Browse all articles >

BROWSE ARTICLES
AUTHOR INFORMATION
Terms of Use   |   Privacy Polity
Editorial Office
Department of Rehabilitation Medicine, Seoul National University Hospital
101 Daehak-ro, Jongno-gu, Seoul, Korea
Tel: +82-10-8678-2671    Fax: +82-2-6072-5244    E-mail: edit@e-arm.org; edit.karm@gmail.com
Business Registration: 110-82-07460                

Copyright © 2024 by Korean Academy of Rehabilitation Medicine.

Developed in M2PI

Close layer