Ann Rehabil Med Search

CLOSE


Ann Rehabil Med > Volume 45(3); 2021 > Article
Rahman, Hanapiah, Nikmat, Ismail, and Manaf: Effects of Concurrent Tasks on Gait Performance in Children With Traumatic Brain Injury Versus Children With Typical Development

Abstract

Objective

To investigate how gait parameters in children with traumatic brain injury (TBI) versus typically developing (TD) children are influenced by secondary concurrent tasks and examine the correlations between gait parameters and attention and balance in children with TBI.

Methods

Sixteen children with TBI (mean age, 11.63±1.89 years) and 22 TD controls (mean age, 11.41±2.24 years) participated in this case-control study. Attention and functional balance were measured using the Children’s Color Trail Test (CCTT) and Pediatric Balance Scale (PBS). All participants first walked without concurrent tasks and then with concurrent motor and cognitive tasks. The APDM Mobility Lab was used to measure gait parameters, including gait velocity, stride length, stride duration, cadence, and double support time. Repeated-measures analysis of variance and Spearman correlation coefficient were used for the analysis.

Results

Children with TBI showed significantly more deterioration in gait performance than TD children (p<0.05). Concurrent tasks (motor and cognitive) significantly decreased gait velocity and cadence and increased stride time; the differences were more obvious during the concurrent cognitive task. A moderate correlation was found between gait parameters (gait velocity and stride length) and CCTT-2 and PBS scores in children with TBI.

Conclusion

Gait performance may be affected by task complexity following TBI. Attention and balance deficits caused deterioration in gait performance under the concurrent task condition in children with TBI. This study illustrates the crucial role of task demand and complexity in dual-task interference.

INTRODUCTION

Children with traumatic brain injury (TBI) typically experience major developmental disruptions and long-term functional impairments [1]. TBI has more severe and persistent effects in children than adults, presumably due to the immaturity of the child’s brain and the potential for early injury, which interferes with ongoing brain development and disrupts related cognitive functions [2,3]. A recent review reported that 50% of children showed some form of cognitive impairment following TBI [4]. Cognitive function deteriorates after TBI, particularly attention, memory, processing speed, and executive function [5]. Studies of the prevalence of motor impairment in children with TBI are particularly limited, as prior studies focused on the rate of motor recovery based on functional rather than impairment measures. However, the majority of children with TBI have disrupted balance performance and walking ability that limit their activities [6,7]. Moreover, they are more prone to gait instability and repeated head injury in the presence of both motor impairments and cognitive deficits [8]. Cognitive and motor function are significantly linearly correlated in individuals with TBI [9]. Most children with TBI present spatiotemporal deviations in gait, including decreased gait velocity, decreased step length, a broad-based gait, and increased double-limb support time compared to typically developing (TD) children [6,7]. The potential consequences of gait impairments in children with TBI include increased disability and increased risk of falls [8]. These limitations can cause TBI children to become more dependent on their caregivers and reduce their community and social participation, reducing their quality of life [10-12]. Thus, recovery toward independent and safe walking is crucial for children with TBI to ensure that they can be independent at home and in the community.
Walking is a fundamental function of mobility in daily life that requires various forms of concurrent motor and cognitive tasks. Performing a dual-task while walking requires some degree of attention and complex processes that involve the ongoing integration of visual, proprioceptive, and vestibular sensory information [13]. The ability to divide one’s attention and process information is also essential in the simultaneous execution of two or more tasks [14]. As attention is a common impairment among children with TBI [15], the demands for attention for tasks may be overwhelming and reduce their performance of one or all tasks [16]. Hence, children with TBI require more attention to walk and are more affected by concurrent tasks.
A previous study of ambulatory children with TBI found that a concurrent cognitive task deteriorated their gait since it imposed high demands on attention [17]. The authors concluded that increasing the complexity level of the task resulted in significant attention-related gait changes and that the existing attention deficit can cause severely disturbed walking ability in children with TBI. Katz-Leurer et al. [17] observed the gait performance of children with TBI who were challenged with concurrent cognitive tasks without concurrent motor tasks. Another recent study showed that cognitive and motor tasks deteriorated gait performance in children with TBI relative to TD children during the Timed Up and Go test [18]. However, the author emphasized that the concurrent motor activity in that study required less effort and was not difficult for those children with TBI who had no motor impairments of the upper limbs and recommended increasing the difficulty of the task in future studies to challenge gait performance. Since walking involves concurrent motor and cognitive tasks in everyday life, it is necessary to evaluate its impact on these children to provide useful knowledge and important outcomes for clinicians who treat children with TBI.
Therefore, the present study aimed to investigate gait performance under concurrent tasks in children with TBI versus TD controls by increasing the complexity level of concurrent motor tasks. The second objective was to compare these two types of concurrent tasks (cognitive vs. motor) on gait performance between groups. Finally, the third objective was to examine the correlations between gait parameters and attention and balance in children with TBI. We hypothesized that the concurrent motor and cognitive task conditions would cause significant gait deterioration compared to the single task condition in children with TBI and that concurrent cognitive tasks would result in greater gait deterioration than concurrent motor tasks in children with TBI.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Participants

This case-control study included 16 children with TBI (13 boys and 3 girls) and 22 TD children (18 boys and 4 girls). Based on a previous study by Katz-Leurer et al. [17], an estimated 30-participant sample would provide 80% power with a type 1 error risk of 0.05. Children with TBI were recruited using purposive sampling from a government-funded hospital, while TD children were recruited from a local school and then matched by age, sex, body weight, and height to the TBI participants.
The inclusion criteria were as follows: (1) at least 6 months post-TBI, (2) Glasgow Coma Scale (GCS) score ≤12 at first admission, (3) age 8–14 years old, (4) ability to walk independently without walking assistance (foot orthoses permitted), (5) ability to obey one-step instructions, (6) ability to carry a tray, (7) full GCS score at the time of enrollment, and (8) provided consent to participate. Participants were excluded if they (1) had received botulinum toxin, (2) had undergone orthopedic surgery in the previous 6 months, (3) had visual field defects, or a (4) had a T-score >75 for disruptive behavior as measured by the Conners Clinical Index. Conners Clinical Index is commonly used in pediatric clinical and research settings to measure behavioral function [19]. It is intended to provide a concise overview of the problems and conditions of children and teenagers, primarily to assess children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder [19]. The study was approved by the Research Ethics Committee, Universiti Teknologi MARA (REC/86/15), and written informed consent was obtained from all participants and their guardians (or legal counsel).

Testing procedure

After written consent was provided, the participants’ demographic data were recorded. Their cognitive function and functional balance were measured using the Children’s Color Trail Test (CCTT) and Pediatric Balance Scale (PBS), respectively. The CCTT is an instrument used for neuropsychological assessments that provides information about visual attention, scanning, processing speed, and mental flexibility and performance [20,21]. The test consists of two sections, CCTT-1 and CCTT-2. The direct score of each section is represented by the time of task completion [20]. In the CCTT-1, the participants connect numbers in circles with alternate colors (yellow and pink) consecutively, while in the CCTT-2, the participants connect numbers in circles with alternate colors consecutively, as the same numbers exist in different colors [22]. The study revealed that the CCTT has a moderate test-retest reliability coefficient range (rtt=0.46–0.68) and moderate to high interference reliability coefficient range (rtt=0.75–0.78) among children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder [23].
The PBS consists of 14 items assessing balance during different activities. On a 5-point scale (0 to 4), each item in the PBS is rated with a maximum total score of 56 (higher scores imply better balance) [24]. The study reported excellent test-retest (intraclass correlation coefficient [ICC]=0.998) and inter-rater reliability (ICC=0.998) when applied to school-aged children with mild to moderate motor impairments [24].
The APDM Mobility Lab (APDM Inc., Portland, OR, USA) is a system used to measure postural sway, postural transitions, and trunk and upper and lower limb movements using inertial sensors connected to particular parts of the body [25]. This system provides gait and balance information. In this study, to record spatial-temporal gait parameters, three motion sensors called “Opal” consisting of a 3-axis accelerometer, gyroscope, and magnetometer (APDM Mobility Lab) were used. The system was portable and user-friendly; therefore, it could be easily set up in a more natural environment than a laboratory. Each Opal was attached to each participant: one on each ankle and one on the lower back. The test using the instrumented long walk consists of walking in a straight line for a distance of 7 m. The primary outcomes obtained from the APDM Mobility Lab system were gait velocity, stride time, stride length, cadence, and double support time to complete the gait task.
The children were asked to walk at their normal pace in a straight line for each task and prioritize the secondary tasks during the concurrent task to magnify their impacts on gait performance. Start and stop signs were positioned on the floor to provide visual feedback to the participants. A trained evaluator provided standardized verbal instructions about the test protocol along with a visual demonstration of the walking test before the test was conducted. To familiarize themselves with the test, the participants performed three practice trials for each task. The task sequence was randomly determined, and the participants were given 3 minutes to rest between tasks. During the test, the participants wore their usual footwear (and orthotics, if applicable).
The concurrent motor and cognitive tasks were adapted from a previous study by Cherng et al. [26]. Since a recent study [18] studied subjects’ performance of a simple concurrent task consisting of carrying an empty tray and counting forward while walking and suggested an increase in the degree of difficulty, we chose the task involving a higher level of difficulty in which the subject carries a tray with seven marbles and counts backward. The test consisted of three different conditions in the following order.
(1) During the single task condition, the participants walked in a straight line for 7 m only (without a secondary task). The participants stood on the start sign comfortably and the experimenter gave the following instruction: “When I say ‘go,’ please walk toward the stop sign at your normal speed.” This procedure was repeated three times.
(2) For the concurrent motor task condition, the participants stood on the start sign comfortably while holding a tray with seven marbles on it (the tray was 17 cm in diameter and 1.2 cm deep, while the marbles were each 2 cm in diameter). When the experimenter said go, the participants walked 7 m toward the stop sign at their normal speed. The participants were required to prioritize the secondary task (carrying the tray) while walking. This procedure was repeated three times. Trials were considered failed and then repeated when the participants dropped one or more marbles from the tray.
(3) For the concurrent cognitive task, the participants had to walk in a straight line for 7 m while repeating a sequence of numbers backward. The task was adapted from the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children’s digit span task. The series of numbers to be replicated was set at the capacity of each child, which was individually decided prior to the experiment by following the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children section’s administration protocol. The participants stood on the start sign comfortably and the experimenter verbally gave them a series of numbers when the participant was ready to start walking. When they heard “go,” they walked 7 m toward the stop sign at their normal speed. The participants were required to prioritize the secondary task (counting backwards) while walking. This procedure was repeated three times. Trials were considered failed and then repeated when the participants committed errors in the counting backwards task.

Statistical analysis

The primary outcomes obtained from the APDM Mobility Lab system were gait velocity, stride time, stride length, cadence, and double support time to complete the gait task. We averaged three trials for each condition. All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS Statistics for Windows version 23.0 (IBM, Armonk, NY, USA). Descriptive statistics and normality of the distribution of variables were determined using skewness and kurtosis tests for all outcome variables. The independent t-test was used to compare the demographic data of the two groups of children. Repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed to analyze the gait parameters across three different conditions (single, concurrent motor, and concurrent cognitive task) and two groups (TBI and TD children). A post-hoc Bonferroni comparison was conducted when the repeated-measures ANOVA test showed a significant difference (p<0.05). Finally, the correlations between the gait parameters and attention and balance performance in children with TBI were evaluated using Spearman correlation coefficient, where 0–0.30, was perceived as a weak correlation, 0.30–0.70 as a moderate correlation, and >0.70 as a strong correlation.

RESULTS

Demographic variables

The characteristics of the participating children are summarized in Table 1.

Gait velocity

The gait velocity of the children with TBI was significantly slower than that of the TD children (group effect, p=0.035) (Fig. 1A). More importantly, concurrent tasks (motor and cognitive) had a differential effect on gait velocity for both groups (condition×group interaction; p=0.012). Table 2 shows a reduction in gait velocity for both groups in the concurrent task but was more significant in children with TBI. The post-hoc analysis (Table 3) revealed that, for children with TBI, the concurrent motor and cognitive task condition led to a slower gait velocity than the single task condition (p=0.005, p<0.001). Similar to children with TBI, both the concurrent motor and concurrent cognitive task conditions for TD children led to slower gait velocity than the single task condition (p=0.001, p<0.001). Although the gait velocity for both groups seemed slowest during the concurrent cognitive task out of the three conditions, it did not differ significantly between the concurrent cognitive task and the concurrent motor task in either group (p=1.000 in TBI, p=0.063 in TD).

Stride time

The reduction in gait velocity may have resulted from the longer stride time, shorter stride length, or both. To determine which gait parameters (spatial or temporal) deteriorated the gait velocity, the stride time and stride length were analyzed. As presented in Table 2, stride time was longer in children with TBI than in TD children (group effect, p=0.042). The stride time was also influenced by the concurrent task, which was confirmed by the significant condition effect (p<0.001). The concurrent task had a similar effect in both groups as confirmed by the non-significant interaction (p=0.051). The posthoc comparisons (Table 3) indicated that the concurrent cognitive task led to a significantly longer stride time than the single task condition (p<0.001) and the concurrent motor task condition (p=0.002). The concurrent motor task increased the stride time slightly compared to the single task, but the difference was non-significant (p=0.03).

Stride length

The stride length did not differ significantly between children with TBI and TD children (group effect, p=0.3) (Fig. 1C). In addition, a significant condition by group interaction (p=0.041) was noted for the stride length parameters despite the reduction trend for each task being similar between groups. Overall, stride length decreased during the concurrent tasks in both groups, but more so in children with TBI. The post-hoc test (Table 3) revealed that the concurrent motor and cognitive task conditions in children with TBI led to a shorter stride length compared to the single task condition (p=0.001, p=0.005), whereas the difference between the concurrent motor and the concurrent cognitive task conditions was nonsignificant (p=0.08). Both concurrent tasks (motor and cognitive) resulted in a significantly shorter stride length than the single task for TD children (p<0.001 for both comparisons). While the concurrent motor task led to a shorter stride length relative to the concurrent cognitive task, the difference was non-significant (p=0.539).

Cadence

The results of cadence during straight walking indicated that children with TBI took fewer steps per minute than TD children (p=0.033). Additionally, the concurrent tasks had a differential effect on cadence as indicated by the significant interaction (p=0.031). The post-hoc analysis (Table 3) revealed that both concurrent tasks resulted in a reduction in cadence compared to the single task in children with TBI (p=0.001 in concurrent motor, p<0.001 in concurrent cognitive). Although the cadence measured during the concurrent cognitive task was lower than that during the concurrent motor task, the difference was non-significant (p=0.086). As for TD children, performing the concurrent motor and cognitive tasks while walking in a straight line caused a decrease in cadence compared to the single task (p=0.001 in concurrent motor, p<0.001 in concurrent cognitive) in TD children. The concurrent cognitive task slightly decreased cadence compared to the concurrent motor task, but the difference was nonsignificant (p=0.058).

Double support time

The double support time during straight walking did not differ between children with TBI and TD children (p=0.29). The concurrent task conditions significantly increased the double support time (condition effect, p<0.001). Moreover, the condition by group interaction was not significant (p=0.29). The post-hoc comparison (Table 3) revealed that the concurrent motor and concurrent cognitive task conditions resulted in significantly higher double support times compared to the single task condition (p<0.001 for both). Although the concurrent cognitive task resulted in a higher double support time than the concurrent motor task, the difference was not significant (p=0.14) for either group.

Correlation between spatiotemporal gait parameters and CCTT-2 and PBS scores in children with TBI

The correlation coefficients between the spatiotemporal gait parameters and the CCTT-2 and PBS scores in children with TBI are shown in Table 4. In children with TBI, the gait velocity for the concurrent motor and cognitive tasks was significantly correlated with the CCTT-2 and PBS scores. For stride length, only the concurrent motor task was significantly correlated with CCTT-2 score. In contrast, all three different task conditions for the stride length parameters were significantly correlated with the PBS score.

DISCUSSION

Deterioration of gait performance while walking in children with TBI was reported previously [7,27-30]. However, there is scant information on the effects of concurrent motor and cognitive tasks on gait performance in children with TBI. The present study investigated the effects of concurrent motor and cognitive tasks on gait performance in children with TBI versus TD controls. The researchers discovered several important findings. First, concurrent tasks (motor and cognitive) adversely affect gait performance in children with TBI and TD controls. Second, children with TBI demonstrated reduced gait velocity, longer step time, and decreased cadence when combined with concurrent tasks relative to the performance of TD children. Third, the concurrent tasks (motor or cognitive), did not have any differential effects on walking performance, other than stride time parameters. Both groups took a longer stride time to perform concurrent cognitive tasks than the concurrent motor task, and this difference was significantly more prominent in children with TBI than in the TD control group. Finally, there was a moderate correlation between gait parameters, specifically gait velocity and stride length, and CCTT-2 and PBS scores.
These study findings demonstrated that children with TBI and TD experienced concomitant changes in gait parameters, including reduced gait velocity, increased stride time, and decreased cadence. Both concurrent motor and cognitive task conditions resulted in the deterioration of gait parameters in both groups relative to the single task condition, but significantly more so in children with TBI. This result suggests that concurrent task conditions were challenging for children with TBI because they have balance and cognitive impairments (PBS: TBI vs. TD, 52.13±5.76 vs. 55.50±0.67 seconds; CCTT-2: TBI vs. TD, 99.50±68.96 vs. 59.64±0.36 seconds). The current findings are in line with those of previous studies that consistently showed deterioration in gait performance in children during walking while performing concurrent tasks [17,18,26,31-33]. They noted that the complexity level of the concurrent task determines the effect on walking performance. Since children with TBI have attention deficits, splitting their attention between two tasks may more greatly interfere with their gait performance than that of the control group [15]. Specifically, they must prioritize their attention to the secondary task and do not have gait as an automatic skill, which may explain the decrease in their gait performance.
In addition, our findings showed that gait deterioration was significantly observed for both groups during the concurrent motor and cognitive tasks compared to the single task, and the concurrent task type (motor or cognitive) did not have differential effects on walking performance. This may be due to the need for more attentional resources than for the single task. This increased deterioration in children with TBI can be explained by the balance deficiency and attention deficit posed in this population as seen in the PBS and CCTT-2 results. This is supported by the correlation analysis, which showed a moderate relationship between spatiotemporal gait parameters, particularly gait velocity and stride length, and PBS and CCTT-2 scores in children with TBI. The combination of balance deficit and attention problems in children with TBI may have made the concurrent tasks (motor and cognitive) more complex, requiring them to move and regulate more body segments while performing tasks at the same time. This requires greater attention compared to a single task. However, a recent study of ambulatory children with TBI showed that concurrent cognitive tasks caused greater deterioration in gait performance than concurrent motor tasks [18]. They concluded that holding an empty tray as a concurrent motor task is less challenging for children with TBI and intact upper-extremity function. Thus, it explained the present study’s finding as the concurrent motor task’s complexity has increased, which required the participant to carry the tray with seven marbles while walking. In contrast, previous studies involving TD children showed that concurrent motor tasks caused a greater reduction in their gait performance than concurrent cognitive tasks [26,32-34]. Furthermore, a study investigating children with developmental coordination disorder (DCD) demonstrated similar findings with TD children in which a concurrent motor task led to greater deterioration in walking under the dual-task condition [31]. These results may be explained by the existence of motor impairments among children with DCD and the lack of a cognitive deficit in TD children, which makes the concurrent motor task more difficult than the concurrent cognitive task.
The results demonstrated a reduction in gait velocity under the concurrent task condition in both groups. Changes in the temporal gait parameters, including stride time and cadence, appear to contribute to the gait velocity reduction. These results indicate that temporal gait parameters may be more vulnerable than spatial gait parameters to concurrent task interference. Clinically, the increase in double support time in children with TBI and balance impairment reflects balance control during gait [7]. Therefore, the increase in stride time led to a decrease in the gait velocity and cadence under the concurrent task condition, reflecting the greater challenge preserving dynamic stability during walking and performing more difficult tasks as a compensatory strategy that would be reflected in the double support time [35].
This study has several limitations. First, the researchers used only one form and one difficulty level of the motor and cognitive tasks. In future studies, when examining the gait of children with TBI in dual-task settings, researchers should use various types and different levels of difficulty of concurrent tasks to assess motor and cognitive capacity. Second, the researchers examined gait only when participants were walking in a straight line. Future studies should investigate the gait characteristics of turning during walking. And finally, the evaluator who performed the APDM Mobility Lab assessments was not blinded to the group allotments. Blindness should be addressed in future studies to avoid potential bias in the findings.
The current study’s findings demonstrated that children with TBI are more vulnerable to dual-task interference during walking than are TD controls. In addition, the concurrent tasks had substantial influence on the temporal gait versus spatial parameters. These findings suggest the importance of dual-task training in the gait training of children with TBI.

CONFLICTS OF INTEREST

No potential conflict of interest relevant to this article was reported.

AUTHOR CONTRIBUTION

Conceptualization: Manaf H. Methodology: Rahman RAA, Manaf H, Hanapiah FA. Formal analysis: Rahman RAA, Manaf H. Project administration: Hanapiah FA, Ismail NA, Nikmat AW. Writing–original draft: Rahman RAA. Writing–review and editing: Nikmat AW, Ismail NA, Hanapiah FA. Approval of the final manuscript: all authors.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This work was funded by the Ministry of Higher Education Malaysia through the Niche Research Grant Scheme (No. 600-RMI/NRGS 5/3 [11/2013]).
The authors are thankful for the support provided by the Research Management Centre, Universiti Teknologi MARA (UiTM), and the participating children and families.

Fig. 1.
Gait parameters under three different task conditions by group: (A) gait velocity, (B) stride time, (C) stride length, (D) cadence, and (E) double support time. p-values of intergroup comparisons under the three different task conditions. TBI, children with traumatic brain injury; TD, typically developing children.
arm-21004f1.jpg
Table 1.
Characteristics of TBI versus TD children
TBI TD p-value
Duration of injury (yr) 2.43±1.48 -
GCS score 9.06±2.29 -
Age (yr) 11.63±1.89 11.41±2.24 0.756
Body weight (kg) 40.64±12.35 41.83±16.93 0.802
Body height (m) 1.415±0.16 1.421±0.15 0.909
BMI (kg/m2) 20.20±5.26 19.90±5.43 0.847
PBS score (max 56) 52.13±5.76 55.50±0.67 0.034*
CCTT-1 (s) 54.94±48.85 33.36±15.27 0.106
CCTT-2 (s) 99.50±68.96 59.64±0.36 0.039*

Values are presented as mean±standard deviation.

TBI, children with traumatic brain injury; TD, typically developing children; GCS, Glasgow Coma Scale; BMI, body mass index; PBS, Pediatric Balance Scale; CCTT, Children’s Color Trail Test.

* p<0.05.

Table 2.
Gait parameters under different conditions in TBI versus TD children
Gait parameter Single task
Concurrent motor task
Concurrent cognitive task
p-value
TBI TD TBI TD TBI TD Intragroup factor Intergroup factor Interaction
Gait velocity (m/s) 1.113±0.19 1.182±0.11 0.999±0.24 1.141±0.10 0.975±0.23 1.106±0.11 <0.001* 0.035* 0.012*
Stride time (s) 1.045±0.10 1.005±0.11 1.136±0.24 1.044±0.10 1.220±0.25 1.070±0.10 <0.001* 0.042* 0.051
Stride length (m) 1.172±0.17 1.199±0.11 1.063±0.20 1.142±0.13 1.110±0.18 1.156±0.12 <0.001* 0.300 0.041*
Cadence (steps/min) 114.63±13.68 121.05±13.58 106.26±16.65 116.95±11.88 101.75±15.67 113.64±12.16 <0.001* 0.033* 0.031*
Double support time (%) 19.894±5.51 18.121±5.80 22.394±7.33 20.555±4.86 23.319±6.10 21.000±5.01 <0.001* 0.290 0.694

Values are presented as mean±standard deviation.

TBI, children with traumatic brain injury; TD, typically developing children.

* p<0.05.

Table 3.
Post-hoc analysis (p-values) of three different task conditions
Gait parameter ST - CMT
ST - CCT
CMT - CCT
TBI TD TBI TD TBI TD
Gait velocity (m/s) 0.005* 0.001* <0.001* <0.001* 1.000 0.063
Stride time (s) 0.03 0.03 <0.001* <0.001* 0.002* 0.002*
Stride length (m) 0.001* <0.001* 0.005* <0.001* 0.08 0.539
Cadence (steps/min) 0.001* 0.001* <0.001* <0.001* 0.086 0.058
Double support time (%) <0.001* <0.001* <0.001* <0.001* 0.14 0.14

ST, single task; CMT, concurrent motor task; CCT, concurrent cognitive task; TBI, children with traumatic brain injury; TD, typically developing children.

* p<0.017.

Table 4.
Correlation between spatiotemporal gait parameter and PBS and CCTT-2 scores in TBI children
Gait parameter CCTT-2
PBS
Spearman rho (ρ) p-value Spearman rho (ρ) p-value
Gait velocity (m/s)
 ST -0.480 0.060 0.460 0.073
 CMT -0.499 0.049* 0.603 0.013*
 CCT -0.555 0.026* 0.507 0.045*
Stride time (s)
 ST 0.132 0.624 0.008 0.976
 CMT 0.344 0.192 -0.276 0.301
 CCT 0.260 0.330 -0.329 0.213
Stride length (m)
 ST -0.355 0.177 0.570 0.021*
 CMT -0.506 0.046* 0.643 0.007*
 CCT -0.400 0.124 0.551 0.027*
Cadence (steps/min)
 ST -0.350 0.184 0.224 0.404
 CMT -0.424 0.102 0.370 0.158
 CCT -0.281 0.292 0.369 0.160
Double support time (%)
 ST 0.241 0.368 -0.429 0.098
 CMT 0.279 0.295 -0.375 0.152
 CCT 0.213 0.427 -0.423 0.103

PBS, Pediatric Balance Scale; CCCT, Children’s Color Trail Test; TBI, children with traumatic brain injury; ST, single task; CMT, concurrent motor task; CCT, concurrent cognitive task.

* p<0.05.

REFERENCES

1. Wetherington CE, Hooper SR, Keenan HT, Nocera M, Runyan D. Parent ratings of behavioral functioning after traumatic brain injury in very young children. J Pediatr Psychol 2010;35:662-71.
crossref pmid
2. Anderson V, Catroppa C. Recovery of executive skills following paediatric traumatic brain injury (TBI): a 2 year follow-up. Brain Inj 2005;19:459-70.
crossref pmid
3. Anderson V, Godfrey C, Rosenfeld JV, Catroppa C. 10 years outcome from childhood traumatic brain injury. Int J Dev Neurosci 2012;30:217-24.
crossref pmid
4. McInnes K, Friesen CL, MacKenzie DE, Westwood DA, Boe SG. Mild Traumatic Brain Injury (mTBI) and chronic cognitive impairment: a scoping review. PLoS One 2017;12:e0174847.
crossref pmid pmc
5. Kohler MJ, Hendrickx MD, Powell-Jones A, Bryan-Hancock C. A systematic review of cognitive functioning after traumatic brain injury in individuals aged 10-30 years. Cogn Behav Neurol 2020;33:233-52.
crossref pmid
6. Beretta E, Cimolin V, Piccinini L, Carla Turconi A, Galbiati S, Crivellini M, et al. Assessment of gait recovery in children after traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj 2009;23:751-9.
crossref pmid
7. Katz-Leurer M, Rotem H, Lewitus H, Keren O, Meyer S. Relationship between balance abilities and gait characteristics in children with post-traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj 2008;22:153-9.
crossref pmid
8. Catena RD, van Donkelaar P, Chou LS. Cognitive task effects on gait stability following concussion. Exp Brain Res 2007;176:23-31.
crossref pmid
9. Sosnoff JJ, Broglio SP, Ferrara MS. Cognitive and motor function are associated following mild traumatic brain injury. Exp Brain Res 2008;187:563-71.
crossref pmid
10. Limond J, Dorris L, McMillan TM. Quality of life in children with acquired brain injury: parent perspectives 1-5 years after injury. Brain Inj 2009;23:617-22.
crossref pmid
11. Souza LM, Braga LW, Filho GN, Dellatolas G. Quality-of-life: child and parent perspectives following severe traumatic brain injury. Dev Neurorehabil 2007;10:35-47.
crossref pmid
12. Rossi C, Sullivan SJ. Motor fitness in children and adolescents with traumatic brain injury. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 1996;77:1062-5.
crossref pmid
13. Woollacott M, Shumway-Cook A. Attention and the control of posture and gait: a review of an emerging area of research. Gait Posture 2002;16:1-14.
crossref pmid
14. Catena RD, van Donkelaar P, Chou LS. The effects of attention capacity on dynamic balance control following concussion. J Neuroeng Rehabil 2011;8:8.
crossref pmid pmc
15. Anderson V, Eren S, Dob R, Le Brocque R, Iselin G, Davern TJ, et al. Early attention impairment and recovery profiles after childhood traumatic brain injury. J Head Trauma Rehabil 2012;27:199-209.
crossref pmid
16. Pashler H. Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. Psychol Bull 1994;116:220-44.
crossref pmid
17. Katz-Leurer M, Rotem H, Keren O, Meyer S. Effect of concurrent cognitive tasks on gait features among children post-severe traumatic brain injury and typically-developed controls. Brain Inj 2011;25:581-6.
crossref pmid
18. Abdul Rahman RA, Rafi F, Hanapiah FA, Nikmat AW, Ismail NA, Manaf H. Effect of dual-task conditions on gait performance during Timed Up and Go test in children with traumatic brain injury. Rehabil Res Pract 2018;2018:2071726.
crossref pmid pmc
19. Gianarris WJ, Golden CJ, Greene L. The Conners’ Parent Rating Scales: a critical review of the literature. Clin Psychol Rev 2001;21:1061-93.
crossref pmid
20. Poreh A, Miller A, Dines P, Levin J. Decomposition of the Trail Making Test: reliability and validity of a computer assisted method for data collection. Arch Assess Psychol 2012;2:57-72.

21. Sanchez-Cubillo I, Perianez JA, Adrover-Roig D, Rodriguez-Sanchez JM, Rios-Lago M, Tirapu J, et al. Construct validity of the Trail Making Test: role of task-switching, working memory, inhibition/interference control, and visuomotor abilities. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2009;15:438-50.
crossref pmid
22. Llorente AM, Sines MC, Rozelle JC, Turcich MR, Casatta A. Effects of test administration order on children’s neuropsychological performance: emerging one-word expressive and receptive language skills. Clin Neuropsychol 2000;14:162-72.
crossref pmid
23. Llorente AM, Voigt RG, Williams J, Frailey JK, Satz P, D’Elia LF. Children’s Color Trails Test 1 & 2: test-retest reliability and factorial validity. Clin Neuropsychol 2009;23:645-60.
crossref pmid
24. Franjoine MR, Gunther JS, Taylor MJ. Pediatric balance scale: a modified version of the berg balance scale for the school-age child with mild to moderate motor impairment. Pediatr Phys Ther 2003;15:114-28.
crossref pmid
25. Mancini M, King L, Salarian A, Holmstrom L, McNames J, Horak FB. Mobility lab to assess balance and gait with synchronized body-worn sensors. J Bioeng Biomed Sci 2011;Suppl 1:007.
pmid pmc
26. Cherng RJ, Liang LY, Hwang IS, Chen JY. The effect of a concurrent task on the walking performance of preschool children. Gait Posture 2007;26:231-7.
crossref pmid
27. Katz-Leurer M, Rotem H, Keren O, Meyer S. The effect of variable gait modes on walking parameters among children post severe traumatic brain injury and typically developed controls. NeuroRehabilitation 2011;29:45-51.
crossref pmid
28. Katz-Leurer M, Rotem H, Keren O, Meyer S. The relationship between step variability, muscle strength and functional walking performance in children with post-traumatic brain injury. Gait Posture 2009;29:154-7.
crossref pmid
29. Katz-Leurer M, Rotem H, Keren O, Meyer S. Recreational physical activities among children with a history of severe traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj 2010;24:1561-7.
crossref pmid
30. Katz-Leurer M, Rotem H, Keren O, Meyer S. Balance abilities and gait characteristics in post-traumatic brain injury, cerebral palsy and typically developed children. Dev Neurorehabil 2009;12:100-5.
crossref pmid
31. Cherng RJ, Liang LY, Chen YJ, Chen JY. The effects of a motor and a cognitive concurrent task on walking in children with developmental coordination disorder. Gait Posture 2009;29:204-7.
crossref pmid
32. Huang HJ, Mercer VS, Thorpe DE. Effects of different concurrent cognitive tasks on temporal-distance gait variables in children. Pediatr Phys Ther 2003;15:105-13.
crossref pmid
33. Huang HJ, Mercer VS. Dual-task methodology: applications in studies of cognitive and motor performance in adults and children. Pediatr Phys Ther 2001;13:133-40.
crossref pmid
34. Hagmann-von Arx P, Manicolo O, Lemola S, Grob A. Walking in school-aged children in a dual-task paradigm is related to age but not to cognition, motor behavior, injuries, or psychosocial functioning. Front Psychol 2016;7:352.
crossref pmid pmc
35. Manaf H, Justine M, Goh HT. Effects of attentional loadings on gait performance before turning in stroke survivors. PM R 2015;7:1159-66.
crossref pmid
TOOLS
Share :
Facebook Twitter Linked In Line it
METRICS Graph View
  • 1 Web of Science
  • 1 Crossref
  • 1 Scopus
  • 3,987 View
  • 110 Download
Related articles in ARM


ABOUT
ARTICLE TYPES

Browse all articles >

BROWSE ARTICLES
AUTHOR INFORMATION
Terms of Use   |   Privacy Polity
Editorial Office
Department of Rehabilitation Medicine, Seoul National University Hospital
101 Daehak-ro, Jongno-gu, Seoul, Korea
Tel: +82-10-8678-2671    Fax: +82-2-6072-5244    E-mail: edit@e-arm.org; edit.karm@gmail.com
Business Registration: 110-82-07460                

Copyright © 2024 by Korean Academy of Rehabilitation Medicine.

Developed in M2PI

Close layer